What would
you pay to be happy?
Alex
Renton Sunday 10 May 2015
theguardian
The
happiest man in the world is a 67-year-old Buddhist monk
calledMatthieu
Ricard. He starts his day sitting in a meadow in front
of his hermitage in Nepal. He watches hundreds of miles of
Himalayan peaks glowing in front of him in the rising sun. The
scene “blends naturally and seamlessly with the peace he has
within”.
Over the
past 40 years Ricard has put in more than 10,000 hours of
meditation: he is the supreme practitioner of what we now call
“mindfulness”. When not gazing at Himalayan peaks, Ricard is likely
to be found in the boardrooms and at the dining tables of the rich
and famous. Ever since his “happiest man” diagnosis in 2008 – made
after days of brain scans at the Laboratory for Affective
Neuroscience at the University of Wisconsin – he has been an object
of fascination for the powerful.
Solving
the problem of human happiness is now firmly on the agendas of
world leaders thanks to Ricard, the scientists who measured his
brainwaves and economists such as Richard
Layard. But their critics say that,
best intentions notwithstanding, in creating happiness science
these pioneers have grown a monster that is already being used to
control humanity and – naturally – make immense riches.
In his
maroon and orange robes, Ricard stands out at the January meetings
of the World Economic Forum in Davos, where he is a regular
participant. He leads meditation sessions there for the captains of
industry, Nobel-winning academics and heads of state before they
start their deliberations, and then joins them to debate the
“Economics of Happiness”.
Ricard
interests the people who run the planet today just as the exotic
animals of far-off lands excited courts and kings in the past. Like
those beasts, his extraordinary mind embodies a prospect of new
wealth and power. Human happiness, an abstract concept and a matter
of philosophical debate since the time of the ancient Greeks, is
now a commodity.
Selling
ideas of happiness is a part of modern life and key to the
marketing of consumer goods from vitamin supplements to cars and
iPhones. But, until now, happiness itself was elusive by
definition. “To be happy, a man must first know what happiness is,”
said Jean-Jacques Rousseau, one of the philosophers who, 250 years
ago, started a debate on the nature and use of happiness that has
not ceased.
Happiness
has been key to considerations of capitalism ever since, in the
19th century, it was first shown that happy workers were more
productive. But the promise of the work done by peering into
Matthieu Ricard’s brain is that Rousseau’s stipulation is becoming
irrelevant. Happiness is no longer an unknown continent. Soon
neuroscience and psychology, in alliance with social economics,
will have located happiness, defined and measured it. And then,
happiness can be sold.
When the
18th-century philosopher Jeremy
Bentham suggested that maximising
happiness was the job of government, he inspired a quest to measure
happiness that continues today. Until recently, the only effective
tool for that – as the political scientist Will Davies explains in
a forceful new book, The Happiness
Industry – has been money. The value of an
object is determined by how much people are prepared to pay for it.
The unpleasantness of a job – grave-digging or rubbish collection –
can be measured in how much people need to be paid per hour to do
it. Governments use these “happiness-measuring”
principles.
When the
US courts were trying to assess what the oil companies should pay
for the Exxon Valdez tanker disaster, which
contaminated a swathe of Alaska, they asked a sample of US
households what they would be “willing to pay” for the accident not
to have occurred. The answer – an average $31 a household – was
used to help calculate Exxon’s ultimate fine. The neo-liberal
economists who have driven conservative political philosophy for 50
years like the simplicity of reducing human feeling to monetary
considerations. But the method was clumsy when used to measure
abstractions such as emotion. However, with the rise of the science
of behavioural psychology, another tool came forward. Economists,
anthropologists and psychologists joined forces in the 1990s,
spurred on by the interest of business and politics.
In
2002, psychologist Daniel
Kahneman won the Nobel prize for
economics for his work measuring experienced happiness. Kahneman’s
research had him at one point measuring the fluctuating emotions of
patients having cameras inserted through their anuses into their
colons. But his findings appeared to say elemental things about the
human condition – not least that people are more motivated by fear
of financial loss than by the possibility of financial gain, to the
extent that they will behave highly illogically. Most of us, for
example, would rather forgo a pay rise if our colleagues are going
to get more than us.
This sort
of insight made politicians and business people sit up and take
notice – and make friends with the new “psycho- economists”.
Richard Layard, a British social economist and associate of
Kahneman, found himself at the top table of Britain’s New Labour
government when it took power in 1997. The press gave him the title
Happiness Tsar, and his 2005 book, Happiness
– Lessons from a New Science, remains a hugely influential
international bestseller. Lord Layard can claim, more than most
social scientists, to have actually changed government policy. His
work on the economics of mental health led him to contend that
modern depressive illnesses were more of a burden to the state than
unemployment. Layard’s “business case” for a new approach was the
engine behind the Labour government’s decision in 2008 to devote
huge resources to tackling depression through new “positive
psychology”, such as cognitive behavioural
therapy.
Since the
early 1970s social economists have discussed the curious
relationship between wealth and happiness.
The Easterlin
Paradox states that while the rich are
happier than the poor within a country, there’s no evidence that
the overall happiness of rich countries is greater than that of
poor ones. Layard and his colleagues’ analysis took this further –
in fact, there seemed to be a wealth ceiling: he put it at around
US$25,000, 10 years ago. Above it increasing wealth showed
diminishing returns – getting richer does not make people
significantly happier. According to more recent research, above
US$75,000 (£48,000) per annum, there’s almost no happiness benefit
at all. The policy implications of that insight were huge,
especially in a world that was beginning to worry about the social
divisiveness of widening inequality.
But the
rulers and the wealthy who wanted to listen to Kahneman, Layard and
Ricard on the social benefits of “mindfulness” – self- regulated
happiness – and measurements of subjective happiness have not leapt
to address superfluous wealth and its proven failure to make even
the wealthy happy. Certainly, in the unhappiest rich countries,
like the US and the UK, taxes on the rich have tended to go down,
not up.
It’s a
failure of which Lord Layard is conscious. “Getting public policy
to focus on making people happy is an ongoing task. We do have
quite powerful evidence that people vote on how satisfied they are
with their lives, rather than on the growth in their income. So we
may begin to see some shift.” His greatest disappointment, though,
is that there has been no shift yet in the way we judge success.
“There’s a very interesting contest going in our society between
the macho culture that puts more and more pressure on people to
compete to get the better of each other, versus the ‘wellbeing’
movement where people get satisfaction from what they contribute to
other people. To change that assumption that that is how life has
to be, and state instead that life is about happiness: that is a
core problem.” Would he welcome a future where science has
succeeded in wholly measuring and defining happiness? “I think that
would be wonderful, so long as it doesn’t stop people living
spontaneously. You don’t want to be checking your happiness watch
all the time.”
Happiness watches, and the
like, are not so far off. Last year’s headline-grabbing tech
gadgetry was wearable fitness and health monitors, delivering data
direct to smartphones. This year gadgets are probing the borders of
happiness itself with technology looking at the brain. The Muse –
$299 in the US, £238 in the UK – is already on the market: a
sensor-laden headband that syncs data to a phone app. It promises
to monitor your brain’s neuron activity and so “improve emotional
state” by training your brain in empathy and composure. This year’s
big launch may be the Thync, a device that promises to
“use neurosignalling to shift your state of mind in a matter of
minutes”. Although no one yet knows what the technology is (all the
company will say is that it intends to “couple energy waveforms to
neural structures”) or even on what body part you’ll wear it, the
Thync has already won a “cool gadget of the year” award. It will be
sold under the slogan “Conquer life!” “Our anxiety is their revenue
opportunity,” says Davies.
Marketing people have long sold new gadgets on threats and promises
about our health and happiness. But there is a new element to this
modern snake oil: it comes with a very exciting secondary revenue
stream. Now your smart bike or your wrist-worn health monitor is
set by default to collect your data and sell it. The mining,
processing and selling of mass data is the trade of the 21st
century. Already it is possible for advertisers to access data that
pertains to mood – Google, notoriously, can offer to target ads to
women who are currently ovulating, a time when significant numbers
of them are more likely to buy clothes, make-up or perfume. The
information is gathered by tracking the monthly rhythms of their
past purchases. Facebook has already conducted experiments in
manipulating the emotions of its users en masse.
As
neurology comes closer to identifying what’s already called the
“buy button” in our brains, mass-data harvesting finds out who to
target at a time when they are most likely to be in a shopping
mood. Meanwhile, marketers use happiness economics and psychology
to find the best way to pitch the item to maximise our desires to
increase our wellbeing. Is it paranoid to see these developments as
a threat to personal privacy – and even to our status as free and
individual humans? Has happiness economics created a monster? Have
corporates used these new sciences to “capture the misery of
working people,” as Davies puts it, to turn more profit?
I asked
Lord Layard if he found anything disturbing about the drive to
monetise happiness, a trade made possible not least by his own
work. He bristled. “I cannot see a risk. If companies understood
better how to help people be happy that would be a very good
thing,” he said. But he did concede that there was a problem if
corporates exploited the flaw in our pleasure-seeking instincts
that makes us grab instant satisfaction.
Davies thinks that the
likes of Lord Layard have already lost control of the monster they
have unleashed. What he calls the “neuroeconomic prejudice” will
ensure that the mathematical, mechanised view of the mind will
triumph, simply because of the financial rewards of an objectively
defined happiness. “Positive psychologists and happiness
economists,” he writes, “make a great play of the fact that money
and material possessions don’t lead to an increase in our mental
wellbeing. But these experts are in a minority, compared with the
vast assemblage of consumer psychologists, consumer neuroscientists
and market researchers all dedicated to ensuring that we do achieve
some degree of emotional satisfaction by spending
money.”
The day happiness is nailed down to a certain amount of gamma wave
activity in the left prefrontal cortex, or a moral decision spurred
by an oxytocin surge, is when something elementally mysterious in
humans becomes a mere mechanism, like the workings of the heart or
bladder. Already many emotions have been “medicalised” – not, some
who work in the field say, in order to help patients, but to sell
drugs. Many writers in the field see the mystery of happiness as
something that must be preserved, like the Green Belt or the
pristine Antarctic.
Tom Cotton
is a psychotherapist working at the frontline of unhappiness in
urban Britain. He fears that techniques espoused by the happiness
economists risk reducing human experience and the mind to the
mechanical, all for reasons of political expediency. “By
definition, each person’s experience – and the meaning they derive
from it – is unique. Psychology, mindfulness and pharmaceuticals
can all potentially be helpful, if this is kept in mind. But if you
use these things to deny or suppress the ups and downs of normal
life experience – which includes things like anxiety and depression
– you store up all sorts of trouble.” The result, he warns, could
be a “covert cultural brainwashing”. “We may conjure up a much
deeper anxiety about what inevitably becomes seen as ‘the
irrational and alien’, both within us, and between ourselves and
others. History makes us painfully aware that this is where
totalitarianism gets a foothold.”
Davies
agrees. The project that began as an attempt to understand how
humans flourish has become, he writes, “a route to sell people
stuff they don’t need, work harder for managers who don’t respect
them and conform to policy objectives over which they have no
say.”
It may be
too late to restore happiness as an elemental mystery. The forces
that want to box it and sell it are the most powerful humanity has
ever known. But there is one protest left to us. It comes from one
of the 20th century’s greatest pleasure-seekers, the poet Guillaume
Apollinaire: “Now and then,” he advised, “it’s good to pause in our
pursuit of happiness and just be happy.” That’s worth a
T-shirt.